09 May 2013

V5 Region of Visual Cortex Responsible For Tracking Fast Moving Object


In a study conducted by UC Berkeley scientists, research shows that for the brain to process fast moving objects, the V5 region of the visual cortex accurately predicts where and how these objects move.

The part of the brain responsible for processing visual information is the Visual Cortex. It can be found in the back of the brain in the Occipital Lobe. Each hemishphere of the brain has its own visual cortex with the right one processing signals from the left visual field and the left one processing signals from the right visual field.

The visual cortex can be grouped into 5 regions; V1, V2, V3, V4, and V5. Each region is responsible for a specific role in visual processing such as spatial recognition and information (V1), long term and short term visual memory (V2), processing of motion- coherent and global (V3) color and shape recognition (V4) and tracking of moving objects (V5).

Finding out specific roles of each region is still an ongoing process and the recent UC Berkeley study show that the V5 region is important in tracking fast moving objects where the speed of the object may be faster than how the brain processes the information.

The V5 region of the visual cortex is also known as the Middle Temporal (MT). This region is believed to be responsible for the processing of motion and moving objects.

Visual Cortex and Fast Moving Objects

How does San Francisco Giants slugger Pablo Sandoval swat a 95 mph fastball, or tennis icon Venus Williams see the oncoming ball, let alone return her sister Serena's 120 mph serves? For the first time, vision scientists at the University of California, Berkeley, have pinpointed how the brain tracks fast-moving objects.

The discovery advances our understanding of how humans predict the trajectory of moving objects when it can take one-tenth of a second for the brain to process what the eye sees. That 100-millisecond holdup means that in real time, a tennis ball moving at 120 mph would have already advanced 15 feet before the brain registers the ball's location. If our brains couldn't make up for this visual processing delay, we'd be constantly hit by balls, cars and more.

Thankfully, the brain "pushes" forward moving objects so we perceive them as further along in their trajectory than the eye can see, researchers said.

"For the first time, we can see this sophisticated prediction mechanism at work in the human brain," said Gerrit Maus, a postdoctoral fellow in psychology at UC Berkeley and lead author of the paper published to be published May 8 in the journal, Neuron.

A clearer understanding of how the brain processes objects in motion can eventually help in diagnosing and treating myriad disorders, including those that impair motion perception. People who cannot perceive motion cannot predict locations of objects and therefore cannot perform tasks as simple as pouring a cup of coffee or crossing a road, researchers said.

Video: Brain Imaging and Visual Cortex

This study is also likely to have a major impact on other studies of the brain. Its findings come just as the Obama Administration initiates its push to create a Brain Activity Map Initiative, which will further pave the way for scientists to create a roadmap of human brain circuits, as was done for the Human Genome Project.

Using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) Gerrit and fellow UC Berkeley researchers Jason Fischer and David Whitney located the part of the visual cortex that makes calculations to compensate for our sluggish visual processing abilities. They saw this prediction mechanism at work, and their findings suggest that the middle temporal region of the visual cortex known as V5 is computing where moving objects are most likely to end up.

For the experiment, six volunteers had their brains scanned, via fMRI, as they viewed the "flash-drag effect," a two-part visual illusion in which we see brief flashes shifting in the direction of the motion.

"The brain interprets the flashes as part of the moving background, and therefore engages its prediction mechanism to compensate for processing delays," Maus said.

The researchers found that the illusion -flashes perceived in their predicted locations against a moving background and flashes actually shown in their predicted location against a still background - created the same neural activity patterns in the V5 region of the brain. This established that V5 is where this prediction mechanism takes place, they said.

In a study published earlier this year, Maus and his fellow researchers pinpointed the V5 region of the brain as the most likely location of this motion prediction process by successfully using transcranial magnetic stimulation, a non-invasive brain stimulation technique, to interfere with neural activity in the V5 region of the brain, and disrupt this visual position-shifting mechanism.

"Now not only can we see the outcome of prediction in area V5," Maus said. "But we can also show that it is causally involved in enabling us to see objects accurately in predicted positions."

On a more evolutionary level, the findings reinforce that it is actually advantageous not to see everything exactly as it is. In fact, it's necessary to our survival:

"The image that hits the eye and then is processed by the brain is not in sync with the real world, but the brain is clever enough to compensate for that," Maus said. "What we perceive doesn't necessarily have that much to do with the real world, but it is what we need to know to interact with the real world."

RELATED LINKS

University of California - Berkeley
Neuron
MIT News: Astrocyte Brain Cells Plays Key Role In Processing Sensory Information
Understanding How The Brain Spatially Represents Object and Action Categories Through fMRI
How Our Brains Keep Us Focused
Making The Brain Sharper and Overcoming Attentional Blink
Darkness Therapy In Treatment of Visual Brain Disorders Like Amblyopia
Researchers Study Visual Intelligence Through Facebook Users